Book Review: Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect by Matthew
Lieberman (Crown Publishers 2013 - Kindle Edition)
This book explores the fact that we are a social species
with social needs, who experience social pain and pleasure, and constantly develop
and utilize social strategies. The author calls his and his colleagues’ work – social cognitive neuroscience – and it
derives from both neuroscience and social psychology. It is a very good book, perhaps
a bit slow and speculative in parts, but it does offer some practical ideas as
well as new ways of understanding human sociality. Lieberman is a psychologist.
This is good cutting edge social psychology with serious potential for practical
applications.
Apparently, neurologically, social pain is similar to
physical pain. Another social issue is that we tend to be influenced by the
reactions of others, even if it against our better judgment. This, the author
says, is due to our social reasoning which appears to exist independently of
what we normally think of as our reasoning faculty. Not only do these appear to
function independently, but when one is engaged the other is disengaged. Indeed
our ability to socialize and to reason socially may be a key to our success as
a species. A goal of this book is to lead toward optimization our social
interactions by taking into account new insights into sociality. Evidence
suggests that paying attention to social well-being within a group leads to the
success of the group. It is a key to teamwork. The author explains that due to
social evolution our well-being is tied to our degree of social connectedness.
Our social strategies derive from our ability to assess others and to read
their intentions. This “mindreading” seems to be unique to primates.
Harmonizing allows us to be influenced by others. These three social
adaptations: connection, mindreading, and harmonizing seem to occur in humans in succession with connection
occurring in infancy, mindreading appearing among toddlers, and harmonizing
among pre-teens. Connection is a feature of mammals, mindreading of primates,
and harmonizing of humans. The last part of the book seeks practical
applications of sociality to make us smarter, happier, and more productive.
The technology of PET scans and now newer techniques allow
neuroscientists to determine which parts of the brain are activated when we are
performing certain tasks. Another unknown is what parts of the brain are activated when the person is at
rest, or in the “default mode network.” Evidence is strong that our default
network is one of social cognition where we think about our relationships with
others. The author thinks this “default social cognition” is automatic, or
built-in, more like a reflex action rather than one selected or willed. The
network, or neural habit, is likely a cause rather than a consequence of our
social activity. Brain studies indicate that it appears in infants and is
finely honed before the teen years. This network is activated in our free time,
when we are not otherwise engaged.
The size of our brains, particularly the size of our
prefrontal cortex, is one of the things that distinguishes us from other
animals. Cognitive functions seem to involve the lateral (outer) brain areas
while social cognition seems to involve the medial (midline) areas. This
suggests, says the author, that social intelligence is separate from general
intelligence rather than a random aspect of it.
And, as stated previously, it seems that when one is activated on the
other is deactivated. These two systems, general cognition and social
cognition, do not feel or seem different to us, but brain area activation
suggests that they are. Autism and its milder form, Asperger’s syndrome,
involve deficits in social cognition and social behavior, but not in cognitive
abilities which evidence suggests can be better in these people. Intelligence
and social intelligence are quite often seen as distinct. This may have to do
with them being different systems. The author suggests that it was not only our
cognitive and analytical needs and skills that made our brains bigger but also
our social needs and skills. Much of our learning is by imitation, thus it is
social learning. Recent evidence suggests that the most compelling reason the
brain grew larger was to so that primates could be more socially active and
live in larger groups. The evolutionary upside was protection from predators,
the downside was more competition for food and sex. Those with the best social
skills could best succeed in such competitive environments where brute force
was restricted to a great extent. Chimpanzees
form alliances and must keep track of social statuses and relationships which
requires brain power. This is true for us humans too. For example, it often
matters not so much that we get endorsed or dissed but who does the endorsing
or dissing.
Fear of public speaking can be seen as fear of social pain,
which in many ways is equivalent to fear of physical pain, as we can react
similarly. For many of us, it hurts to be rejected.
Part of brain development takes place in the womb but much
of it takes place after birth while being immersed in our human culture. An
undeveloped brain at birth means that infants can’t survive on their own and we
have the longest period of immaturity of any mammalian species. All mammals
need a caregiver after birth to meet their biological needs – so perhaps, says
the author, there is another rung on the bottom of Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs
– that being social contact with a caregiver.
Social pain has no location but if one analyzes physical
pain in detail the sensory information is often distorted – there is a
psychological component to it as well. Social distress can be as debilitating
as physical pain. Our metaphorical use of pain terminology to describe social
distress is perhaps very revealing in this regard – ie. broken hearts and hurt
feelings.
Psychologists in the 1950’s came up with the idea of attachment to describe the distress
displayed by infants when separated from their caregivers. Babies cry, or give
distress calls when separated from a caregiver. This attachment distress is
distinctly social, says the author. Thus our connections to our primary
caregivers make up our first social connections. “Staying connected to a
caregiver is the number one goal of an infant.” In the 1950’s during the heyday
of Behaviorism the attachment between infant and caregiver was thought to be
due to associative learning and in a trend that accelerated in the 1990’s due
to better neurological evaluation techniques, these days the biological
explanations of relationships are catching up and passing cultural explanations. In this case, though,
an experiment in the 50’s with surrogate mothers for monkeys by psychologist
Harry Harlow where the surrogate that seemed more like a monkey was preferred
over one that didn’t but provided milk. In terms of neurochemicals it has been
demonstrated that internal painkillers are activated following union after
separation. Other experiments have shown that the same brain regions are
activated for physical pain and social distress. Experiments were done where
confederates (those in on the experiment) socially rejected the other person by
stopping throwing the ball to them in a game. This was done while the person
was in an fMRI scanner. The people rejected reported feeling social distress
and activated regions of the brain associated with physical pain much more,
particularly the Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex (dACC). Oddly, even after
people were told that they were just playing the game with computer presets
they still felt the social distress. The author compares such an effect to the
effects of optical illusions. Other
experiments showed that the dACC performed two closely related cognitive
functions: error detection and conflict monitoring. The author and his wife,
psychologist Naomi Eisenberger, did experiments and wrote papers characterizing
the dACC as having a social function as an alarm system which utilizes its
function of error detection. The alarm part is an emotional component while the
detection part is a cognitive component of the overall function of the dACC,
say the authors. “Sticks and stones will hurt my bones, but names will never
hurt me?” Not true, says the author. “Our sensitivity to social rejection is so
central to our well-being that our brains treat it like a painful event ...”
Bullies take advantage of this. Most bullying involves social rejection rather
than physical aggression. Did our need to grow and develop our brain outside
the womb lead to our susceptibility and sensitivity to social pain and
rejection? It seems so.
Fair vs. unfair treatment is explored as a social phenomenon.
Offers deemed unfair are typically rejected even if the result is less than the
unfair offer. When we are treated fairly we feel that others value us and that
the social relationship is good. It is a sign of social connection. Fairness
reinforces our connection to another or to a group and activates what is known
as the reward system in the brain. Thus, fairness is a social reward, very much
the opposite of social pain. Another thing that triggers social reward is our
sense of belonging. Money is a social
reward as we get paid to do something for others. It is what has been called a
‘secondary reinforcer,’ the primary reinforcers being basic needs like food,
water, shelter, heat, etc. Money is a secondary means to make sure that basic
primary needs are met. Social regard, he suggests, can act as both primary
and/or secondary reinforcement. We desire social regard and work to secure it.
Humans are super-cooperators and this cooperation allows us to do vastly more
together than separately. One way we cooperate is through the principle of
reciprocity, one of our strongest social norms. When we receive a gift of any
sort we feel obligated to reciprocate. Salespeople can exploit this situation.
According to standard principles of economics cooperation appears irrational.
Cooperation opposes the self-interest that is human nature, proposed by
philosophers David Hume and Thomas Hobbes. Nowadays we might see cooperation
more in terms of social biology, as social cooperation has increased our
survivability and our tendency to cooperate may reach down to the genetic
level. Experiments with different societies, including hunter-gatherers, have
noted that people often cooperate and so make decisions against their
self-interest. Richard Dawkins would say that cooperation and altruism is
wholly taught and learned while biologist E.O. Wilson would suggest that the
biological component of altruism is potent and is often related to group
natural selection, which he proposes as a companion to individual natural
selection. Others, like Dawkins, think cooperation is ultimately concerned with
self-interest. Our brain’s reward system responds to teaming up and cooperation
which suggests that we do get some individual reward but there may of course be
more to altruism than that.
Altruism is difficult to evaluate because it is difficult to
evaluate the psychological motivations and true intentions of others. However,
some experiments have shown that empathy can be induced and those who have
empathy towards others in an experiment are very likely to be altruistic,
likely authentically. The Dalai Lama notes that the intelligent way to be
selfish is to work for the benefit of others since that is also intrinsically
pleasurable. Giving to charity also preferentially lit up the brain’s reward system,
more so than receiving it. Indeed, neuroscience has shown that both empathy and
generosity are rewarding. The author notes that there are then two kinds of
social rewards: praise from others and helping others. We feel cared for when
we are verbally groomed much like other mammals probably do when they are
physically groomed. The neuropeptide oxytocin has been associated with
caregiving motivations, even overcoming fear in approaching distress. The
author thinks of oxytocin as a kind of neurochemical nurse. Oxytocin may also
be involved in the brain’s reward system. There may also be an aggressive side
to oxytocin related to increased protection of offspring from potential
threats. It seems to depend on whether one sees another as friend/familiar or stranger,
or perhaps in terms of social biology, as in-group or out-group. An odd
phenomenon observed by social psychologists (and many of us regular folk) is
faux-selfishness, where we claim a selfish motivation to hide an unselfish one.
One reason could be that we feel out of place, or abnormal, being unselfish in
a selfish world. We somehow feel we are supposed to choose self-interest over
altruism. We may well assume that others are more self-interested than they
actually are. After a while this behavior may become habitual. We have both
selfish and unselfish motivations. Mammalian brains are wired to care for
others and it has been shown that primates extend caring to some non-kin as
well as kin. Since social connection is reinforced through social pain and
social pleasure mirroring the physical, finding ways to keep socially connected
is the “central problem of mammalian evolution.” Severing a strong social bond
through a breakup with a partner or the death of a loved one can be emotionally
very difficult for us. We tend to long to stay connected to those with whom we
have entwined our lives.
Mindreading, or reading the intentions of others, is the
next subject. Early social psychologists and phenomenologists noticed the
importance of mindreading. Experiments have shown that even when examining the
interaction of objects we have a tendency to convert them into a drama of
beings, changing action into human-like behavior, that is, if they remind us of
humans. Daniel Dennett noted that we innately assume others are intentional
beings. He called this the ‘intentional stance,’ whereby we assume the
intentionality of others. It is quite obvious to us that others have a ‘theory
of mind’ just like us. Frequently, we consider and think about the mental
states of others. This has come to be called mentalizing. Children seem to develop this skill early but do not
start life with it. Chimpanzees do not seem to get it but they do seem to have
precursors to it. Our capacity for mindreading allows us to make social
scenarios and this supports our motivation for connection. Deductive and
inductive reasoning are involved in mindreading. Working memory (the ability to
hold immediate facts) is involved in these reasonings. Certain areas of the
brain light up (lateral prefrontal and parietal regions) when engaged in this mentalizing
activity. The author contends that the brain uses two separate systems for
social vs. non-social thinking. Studies conducted in MRI scanners have
confirmed this. The author refers to those regions of the brain involved as the
‘mentalizing system.’ This mentalizing system coincides quite well with the
default network, involved with social thinking, described earlier. It is
plausible that early hominid groups without language were faced with the need
of hunting in groups and had to coordinate. This likely involved reading the
intentions of others in the group and so the system evolved. Mindreading is a
skill that can be well-developed as poker players can attest. He mentions club
DJs as having to read the intentions of the audience in order to pick the right
music at the right time and notes that with the internet and social media we
all become information DJs. He thinks the mentalizing system basically works
like a social working memory system but still separate from non-social working
memory. Mentalizing requires effort and so we find shortcuts, called heuristics to simplify decision-making.
We often use our own mind as a proxy for other’s minds.
Observations of certain groups of neurons while humans or
primates were performing certain tasks and/or watching others perform tasks,
led to the theory that these are “mirror neurons” – involved in learning by
imitation. While there is currently much debate about whether these mirror neurons
actually exist as such, it can be said, according to the author, that the brain
has a “mirror system” – a series of places that light up when such tasks are
performed and/or watched. The mirror neuron theory argues for a
perceptual-motor overlap. In any case, it appears that observing others can be
perhaps more interactive than we realize and that there is mental mimicry
afoot. Candidates for what accelerated human development circa 50,000 years ago
include an enhancement in our working memory system and a change in mirror
neurons. Building shelters, making tools, and hunting were likely learned by
imitation. Apparently, there are two main theories how we read other’s
intentions: by Theory of Mind we do it through logical inference and by
Simulation we mentally project and recreate the scene in our own imagination.
Mirror neurons might be involved in such recreations though this has not been
proven. Vittorio Gallese, one of the ‘discoverers’ of mirror neurons, thinks
that they work as a “motor resonance” when we simulate. We see someone doing something
and we know how it feels to do just that and part of our mental system is doing
just that. Other scientists disagree. Some say mirror neurons are just
conditioned motor neurons that are involved in perceptual or sensual memory
rather than dedicated to mirroring others. There is much debate and confusion
in the studies. The author compares the mentalizing systems to the mirror
system and concludes that the mirror system has been devised to explain lower
motor functions while the mentalizing system is more involved with reading higher-level
intentions. The author and his colleagues’ experiments suggest that the
mentalizing system is concerned with ‘why’ and the mirror system is concerned
with ‘how.’ They see the mirror system as a sort of precursor to the
mentalizing system. Mentalizing involves more of a mindreading based on words
and so is likely more recent in our evolutionary history than mindreading based
on mental-motor imitation of actions, ie. the mirror system.
Empathy refers to connecting with the experience of another.
The author states that there at least three psychological processes that
coincide to make an empathic state: understanding through mindreading, affect matching, and empathic motivation. If we see a part of a person’s body being hurt
it may draw our attention to that part of our body. Studies have documented
affect matching when the pain distress
network in the dACC of the brain was activated in subjects that merely
watched others being administered painful shocks. People often do not get
beyond mindreading and affect matching and end up turning away from full
empathic responses which require empathic motivation. Experiments by the author
and colleagues have shown that the septal region of the brain is most
associated with empathy and this region has direct connections to the dorsomedial
prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) which seems to control the mentalizing system. The
septal area has been associated with reward, fear regulation, and maternal
caregiving since the 1950’s. These three activities involve approach and
avoidance motivations. Such a regulatory system may be helpful in mammalian
parenting where fear of loud noises needs to be dampened. It has been shown
that damage or removal of septal area components results in an overactive
startle response, reduced fear regulation, and over-reactive responses to fear.
“Empathy is arguably the pinnacle of our social cognitive
achievements – the peak of the social brain.”
Many agree that impairment of Theory of Mind (seeing others
as agents with minds) is a characteristic of autism. It is disagreed whether
this is a cause or consequence of the condition. The author thinks it makes
more sense as a secondary consequence and not a cause. Since the mirror system
occurs in primates but Theory of Mind does not, it is likely older in
evolution. There is now a theory of autism – the Broken Mirror Hypothesis –
that links a damaged mirror system, where social imitation is impaired, to
account for autism and its social deficits. It is hard to know if this is truly
the case as experiment design needs to be foolproof. Apparently, some imitation
is automatic and it is unclear whether autistic persons have deficits in the
ability to imitate or do it too good as hyper-imitators, as studies suggest
both. The author has a suspicion that children with autism are not insensitive
to the social worlds but overly sensitive to it so they come to prefer social
isolation to contact. This is called the – Intense World Hypothesis - for the cause
of autism. The amygdala, which codes for and responds to the emotional
intensity of events in our experience, if out of balance, may lead to
anti-social behavior. Enlarged amygdalas may be hyper-active and are
well-documented in autistic people.
“It suggests that the autistic individual’s aversion to the
social world is a coping mechanism for dealing with the most intense and
unpredictable part of the world (that is, people), which overwhelms them,
literally, in each encounter.”
This may lead to their mentalizing systems being undeveloped
or underdeveloped.
Next examined is our sense of body and the mind/body dualism
represented by Descartes:
“Descartes’ belief about our dual nature – mind and body –
was a profound error about the way nature works, but it was an accurate
assessment of how our brains represent the world.”
Chimps given mirrors have developed a successively a more
detailed and familiar sense of self-recognition in experiments. But seeing oneself
and knowing oneself as oneself are not equivalent. Sense of self has been tied
to the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). The MPFC may be the main brain
structure that separates us from other primates. The author contends that the
sense of self is more or less an imaginary self, presumably the illusion perpetuated
by the MPFC. The author sees it as a ‘trojan horse self.” Nietzsche saw the
self as a social construct and this is yet quite plausible to neuroscientists.
Lieberman suggests that “evolution is moving us ever closer
to independent social living, where we maximize what we can do together in
groups.” This is the social adaptation known as “harmonizing.” Our very sense
of self may be part of such an overall social adaptation strategy. We may well
have a sense of self we project onto and read from group feedback. It has been
called reflected appraisal generation
and can be summarized as “… what I think you think of me.” How others see us
affects us and we are molded socially by society to varying degrees. Prediction of behavior has been shown to be
better from known reflected appraisal than from known direct self-appraisal. Advertisers
have long quested to exploit this situation, attempting to influence our behavior
by influencing how we see others seeing us. We are creatures with both selfish
impulses and social adaptations that seek to optimize social harmony or cohesion.
There is an interesting section on “panoptic self-control”
which is the fact that we are very likely to exhibit extra self-control when we
know we are being observed. Self-control and the ability to delay gratification
tend to be a useful social assets. Social psychologists associate self-control
and social restraint with the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) as the
hub of the brain’s braking system.
Perspective taking and the false consensus effect are examined. The false consensus effect has
to do us erroneously believing that much of the world shares our personal views
about things. We assume that our perspective is more or less universal and very
often it is not. Studies show that we do tend to project our views onto others.
We tend to project our belief biases, or confirmation biases. Successful
perspective taking often requires reappraisal
whereby we reevaluate our perspective to better match reality. Affect labeling where we label and
describe our emotional reactions to things, ie. phobias, is examined as a kind
of implicit self-control.
“Self-control is the price of admission to society” (society
requires restraint)
Self-control and delayed gratification are associated with
success and one’s ability to benefit society and the greater good. Shared
beliefs (mostly about the nature of reality) help shape our social self-control.
For panoptic self-control, experiments have shown that the mere presence of a
mirror activates it, though probably to a lesser extent than if we suspect we
are really being observed. Panoptic self-control is unique to humans.
Practicing self-control allows us to better benefit society or the immediate
group. We prioritize the good of the group over self-interest and this also
enhances social connection. Experiments have shown that we have a tendency to
conform to expected behavior to avoid the social punishment of feeling out of
place. Conformists can be the most motivated people as the society around them
rewards them for conformity. Perhaps it is “sheeple power.” Self-control may
seem to be composed of will-power but another key component is social
conformity.
To summarize: our social brain developed connection reinforced by social pain
and pleasure as a mammalian adaptation to better care for the young. With
primates came mindreading, first in
the imitative form of the mirror system, then in the human mentalizing system,
which is a powerful tool for developing social strategies. Self-knowledge,
though illusory, allows us to practice self-control, which enhances our ability
to harmonize and so to optimize
group behaviors.
Our conceptions of happiness may have more to do with
success in these social adaptations than we realize. Money and consumeristic
goals are limited after “enough” is obtained. There is a phenomena called hedonic adaptation which allows us to
adapt to our situation. If we experience loss we adapt by losing the anxiety
around the loss. If we experience gain we adapt by losing the elation around
the gain. In some sense gain can even be worse than loss as it spurs us to seek
more gain. Social capital can be a key component to our well-being. Studies
indicate that socializing is quite a bit less common than it used to be. The
author suggests investing in social enhancements like transportation
infrastructure and creation of freely available localized social spheres in places
like college dorms, apartment buildings, etc. Socializing seems hampered in our
society by fragmentation of views about things like politics, religion, music,
drugs, food choices, hobbies, etc. Things like TV and internet have tended to
decrease our face-to-face socializing as we choose on-line socializing.
Facebook has made some interesting changes whereby we have gotten somewhat
beyond topic-specific groups and into a more free-for-all social sphere, for
good and/or ill.
Social matters in the workplace and business compensation
can be in more than money. David Rock’s Neuroleadership Institute sees
workplace motivation in his acronym SCARF: status,
certainty, autonomy, relatedness, and
fairness. Studies have shown that people can sometimes prefer being conferred
special status to money. Status, connection, and fairness can positively affect
the bottom line of companies that promote them. The chance to help others is
also a potential social reward that motivates us. The nature of many of our
jobs often does not lend itself to being directly beneficial to others. We are
probably more motivated to work when we feel we are benefitting society. People
tend to want to contribute – to the company they work for and to society at
large – and if they feel that they do they will also be more likely to be a
productive employee. The brain’s reward circuitry is thus activated. Evidence
shows that leaders with strong social abilities succeed and are well-liked.
Social skills improve the value of all the other leadership competencies. Harmonizing
is an important function of team members on work projects.
The author thinks that we can optimize our ability to
harmonize through educating the social brain and a key opportunity is in the “junior
high” years where young teens are often concerned with fitting in and
belonging. The need to belong is often at odds with doing well academically and
young teens can turn away from education. If social connection helps us to
succeed then it is likely that social rejection does the opposite. As many as 40%
of American children report being bullied at one time or another. Social pain
caused by rejection can distract from work and so grades drop. Experiments have
shown that an enhanced sense of belonging can lead to better grades. Less
distraction by social pain and feeling good about one’s sociality may lead to a
more functional working memory. Dopamine may be one reason this is so. The
author thinks that utilizing the social brain during the learning process can enhance
education rather than trying to separate it out. Experiments have revealed a social encoding advantage in learning
where retention is enhanced when thinking of things in a social way rather than
in rote memorization. It appears that this is due to activation of the
mentalizing system. This suggests that the mentalizing system can vastly aid
the development of memory. He thinks History and English need to focus more on
Why people did what they did and Why the rules of English are effective in
communication. A better appreciation of history can be had through utilizing the
mentalizing memory system. Better communication is a goal of learning language
and better communication means better socialization. The “learning-for-teaching”
effect has been shown to enhance learning. If one learns something so that one
can teach it then one has a social motivation for learning – to be a better
conveyor of knowledge, a better teacher, for others. The motivation to share information
can help us to remember, retain, and integrate information better. Peer-tutoring
has been shown to positively affect both giver and receiver. This is another
form of socially motivated learning. In younger grades, if older students can
routinely tutor younger students as part of their own learning then such a
system of tutoring utilizing social motivation can be beneficial, suggests the
author. Such a system may succeed with the help of social reinforcements:
younger students wanting to make a good impression on older students and older
students making sure they know what they are teaching so as not to be
embarrassed by younger students. Classes in social intelligence and exercises
in enhancing sociality might also be useful. These days they may fall under the
subject of psychology.
The mentalizing system is our social imagination that seeks
to optimize our social connectivity, our ability to read others, and to
harmonize with others. Newer neuroimaging techniques such as functional near
infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) may help to reveal our social adaptation systems
in more detail. This and other techniques promise to be less invasive and far less
expensive than MRI scanners. They can transmit neural data to a station while
people are doing different things while wearing them as headbands.
No comments:
Post a Comment