Book Review: The Social Conquest of Earth by Edward O. Wilson
(Kindle Edition 2012)
This is a fascinating foray into gene-culture co-evolution
and multilevel selection which is the interplay of individual and group
selection. It is also a history of eusocial evolution and provides an
unparalleled background of the dynamics of human social evolution. Wilson is an accomplished
biologist, an expert particularly on ant species. Much of the book is a
comparison between humans and eusocial insects. He has also written much about
social evolution through the years. His book – Sociobiology – was criticized
due to the suggestion that some aggressive behaviors like racism may have a
biological or evolutionary component but he has since changed his orientation.
In the introduction Wilson
touches on the human condition through the work of the artist Paul Gauguin. His
12 ft wide masterpiece painted in Tahiti was
called – ‘This painting is not an answer. It is a question.’ Wilson uses this as a starting point on his
foray into discovering what forces of evolution make up human impulses and the
human condition.
The first story bonds of tribes are in the creation myths.
Each of countless clans and tribes of the past shared their creation stories as
a bond. I sort of tend to disagree with Wilson’s
conclusion that myth/religion and science can never be reconciled. Who knows
really? Although I agree that science offers us the best chance for agreed upon
knowing – I think that all knowledge is an approximation. Therefore I more
definitely disagree with his dismissal of philosophy. Luckily it is the science
part that is valuable in this book.
He notes that humans were quite vulnerable to extinction
throughout much of their early history due to many factors: small bands,
droughts, disease, etc. Eusocial insects such as ants, bees, wasps, and
termites have been around for tens or hundreds of millions of years compared to
our meager few million. They evolved at a slower pace and achieved a better
balance with the overall ecosystems while we have come to dominate and threaten
the very planet we inhabit. Humans are also a eusocial species, defined as a
species where group members consist of multiple generations and are prone to
perform altruistic acts as part of their division of labor. This is merely one
similarity to the other eusocial species. There are many obvious differences. Our
eusociality apparently came about in different ways. He lists these as
altruism, cooperation, competition, domination, reciprocity, defection, and
deceit. Insects have size constraints due to their skeletal structures and brain
size constraints. This limited their mental development to the instinctual
level.
Wilson
likens the path of evolution to a maze where old corridors (niches) close while
others open. He talks much about ‘preadaptation’ as predisposition favorable to
precede certain adaptation. An example is the relative size and relative
immobility of humans compared to eusocial insect species. Other ones mentioned
for primates are specialization for life in the trees – large eyes with color
vision, opposable thumbs and fingers for grasping, and things like bipedalism
when life on the ground became prominent. Bipedalism allowed primates to carry
food in the hands for considerable distances. Perhaps, he says, due to their 3D
environment and complex feeding behaviors they began to develop larger brains.
They also began to rely more on vision and less on smell. He states that
bipedalism was key to the success of the prehumans called australopithecines. Three species of them were known in Africa and they travelled in small groups in the manner
of hunter-gatherers. Later homo
species developed long-distance running abilities, dropped body hair, and
developed sweat glands. These adaptations increased survivability in the hot
savanna environment. The ability to throw things, to utilize projectiles, he
thinks is a cultural adaptation. A few chimps do it as well. The next step
toward human eusociality he gives as the control of fire. Perhaps first seeing
and searching for dead animals caught in brush fires and noticing their cooked
flesh easier to remove led to harvesting fire for cooking. Fire allowed the
gathering of groups at campsites. These were probably kin groups along with
women from nearby groups as in modern hunter-gather societies. Campfire sites
were used by most or all of the homo
groups and thought to extend back about 1 million years. The basic similarity
to eusocial insects is that the insects raised young in a nest that they
defended and foraged for food away from the nest.
Homo habilis
(1.8-1.6 mya) developed from australopithecines.
Habilis had evolved teeth to better consume meat and this consumption likely
began as humans ate large herbivores found dead or killed by other predators
that they could chase off. Successful competition for food, the use of tools to
dig roots, and the wile to outwit enemies, likely contributed to better
survival. Increased meat in the diet may have been a major contributing factor
to larger brain size for hominid species. Wilson
suggests that hunting strategizing actually may have been an even bigger factor
in brain size increase. Hunting in groups is rare among mammals, occurring
mostly in our common-ancestored primates (chimps and bonobos), but also among
lionesses, wolves, and wild dogs. Utilization of teamwork to get meat, a denser
proteinated and efficient food source, likely led to bigger brains and
gradually more complex social behavior. They, as well as other eusocial
species, foraged from protected nests. The protected nest was a place to go
from and return to, a place of gathering, or reconvening, which seems conducive
to the development of social behavior. Having both meat hunters and vegetable
gatherers in the group helped to develop division of labor and competition for
greater share of food. Status in the group became more important. The ability
to read the intentions of others was key to social intelligence (probably still
is). Those with this ability could also better outwit and defeat competing
tribes.
Next he lists the major pre-adaptations, each leading to the
next, that lead to modern humans: 1) living on land, 2) large body size
(allowed for a larger brain) compared to other eusocial species, 3) grasping
hands to hold, climb, and manipulate things, unique to primates, allowed for
technological development, 4) adding some meat to the diet, first from
scavenged carcasses, then from hunting, 5) cooperation in harvesting meat which
led to highly organized groups, 6) controlled use of fire which allowed for
evolution of digestion of cooked meat and social sharing of prepared meals.
Fire also lead to – 7) extended base campsites that could be defended like a
nest, 8) division of labor.
Wilson
favors natural selection as the force that allowed us to navigate the
evolutionary maze. He cites his early fascination with kin selection and its
result called inclusive fitness theory. Now he favors group selection rather
than kin selection – the notion that the fitness of the group was the important
factor rather than just the fitness of kin. The interplay of individual
selection and group selection, what he calls multi-level selection, he thinks,
is the natural selection mechanism of humans. This theory is not based on
degrees of relatedness as in kin selection, but is simply an acknowledgement of
the necessity of balancing the needs of the individual and the needs of the
group. Early homo sapiens consisted
of groups competing for resources. The most cooperative groups became the most
successful and the fittest. He sees a tension between the forces of individual
selection, generally self-interested, and group selection, generally
altruistic. The needs of the group came to overcome the needs of the members,
but also to overcome the selfish survival habits of the members. He sees it as
a cost/benefit dynamic.
He thinks this clash of individual and group selection is
responsible for the arising of culture among humans. In this scenario selfishness,
cowardice, and hypocrisy are products of individual selection and virtue,
honor, and duty are products of group selection.
Based on the value of group selection in determining group
fitness, Wilson
concludes that: “tribalism is a fundamental human trait.” Our group or tribal
instincts are a product of group selection. Modern humans exist within a social
world of multiple interlocking tribes with various focuses. He gives the
fanaticism around team sports as an example of tribalism. Tribal members
develop in-group or home team preferences. Wilson thinks we have some brain hardwiring
to favor groupishness. Tribalism is perhaps a key to humanity’s history and
practice of warfare. Wilson mentions the modern
example of Rwanda
where in 1994, about 800,000 people were slaughtered by knife and gun in about
100 days. Wilson
emphasizes the warlike nature of humans and suggests our societies are
hopelessly bound to war behavior as history has shown – but I tend to think we
can evolve beyond such limitations. Tribal mortality rates through war and
homicide among hunter-gatherers, past and present, and even among chimps are
remarkably similar. One thing is certain – territorial aggression and
protection have a vast history. Population growth and competition for resources
can lead to aggression. He likens these to limiting factors affecting hierarchical
populations in the food chain. Limiting factors tend to kick in instinctual
behaviors in various species such as emigration or can be natural limiting
factors such as starvation due to overgrazing after removal of a predator. The
advent of Neolithic agriculture allowed much greater populations to be
sustained but war and tribal instinct did not abate between differing tribes.
Our tendency to ‘multiply and consume’ has brought us to the brink of biosphere
collapse in modern times.
Homo erectus was possibly buffered against
extinction by its great range. From 1.5 mya this ancestor of ours roamed much
of the earth excluding Australia,
the New World, and various islands. Homo
sapiens developed diagnostic features rare among animals: use of language,
music, prolonged childhood, concealed female genitalia and female ovulation,
fast brain growth during early development, small teeth and slender body suited
to an omnivorous diet, and a digestive system suited to eat food tenderized by
cooking. Wilson also gives a history of the
spread of anatomically modern humans out of Africa.
After this exodus some 48-60,000 years ago there was an explosion of culture
and creativity. There are 3 main hypotheses for this creativity: mutation, a
gradual development that happened to culminate in this time period, and that
cultural innovations occurred then relapsed as deteriorating climate caused
shrinking population and hardship – then re-emerged in newer forms as climate
improved. All three ideas may be reconciled. Wilson suggests this time period as one where
human culture became autocatalytic – where one innovation made others possible.
These innovations increased group fitness and survivability. I guess this would
be the time when cultural evolution gained on and perhaps even later overtook
purely biological evolution for our species. The biggest overall jump may have
come in the Neolithic 10,000 years ago with the advent of agriculture. With it
came food surpluses, trade, villages, and specialization. Wilson suggests that the roots of agriculture
came much earlier – 45,000 years ago when game was driven and captured by
starting fires. As a result of this they likely noticed that growth of certain
plant species was enhanced after the fires and so deliberate burning and selection
of certain plants enhanced by burning may have been practiced. This eventually
led to plant and tree domestication which occurred close to the same time as
agriculture and animal domestication. Fast-forwarding to modern times Wilson gives his opinion
of artificial intelligence as possible on certain levels of memory,
computation, and information processing – but unlikely to replace the
magnificent human mind, our treasured biological heritage.
Among humans anthropologists
recognize three levels of societies: generally egalitarian hunter-gatherer
bands, chiefdoms, and states. These developed culturally rather than
biologically and are based on size, expansion, degree of success in war, and
resource availability. Hierarchies develop in more complex social systems for
reasons of efficiency and order. Studies have found that variation in
personality traits within an ethnic group is relatively consistent when
comparing different groups. This suggests that while there are general
differences in traits between ethnicities there is also variability within and
so the effects of culture are stronger than the effects of biology in making
people different. Similarity of genetic dispositions among differing peoples
likely led to them coming to the same adaptations (ie. state formation,
agriculture, animal domestication, and possibly writing) in different places
independently of one another. These
happened at different times and speeds. According to Jared Diamond’s famed
work, Guns, Germs, and Steel, the
reason for this was chiefly geography and availability of resources.
Part III of the book covers the
development of social insects. He notes that eusociality is rare. Protected
dwellings with multigenerational inhabitants and outwardly altruistic division
of labor, are features of eusociality. These adaptations give social insects
their ability to dominate the earth in terms of sheer numbers. Leafcutter ants
have complex social behaviors. They practice agriculture by using their feces
as fertilizer to grow a specific fungus which feeds them. Other ants farm
aphids, scale insects, and mealybugs much like humans farm dairy cows living
mainly off of their honeydew (excrement and excess moisture), only killing them
in famine times. The ants actually protect these sapsuckers from other
predators. The ant/sapsucker symbioses are widespread and contribute much to
overall ecosystem cohesiveness. Eusocial insects developed innovative symbiotic
relationships to their environment that helped them succeed. The change in
plant dominance from gymnosperms to angiosperms (seed producing plants) increased the survivability of those
angiosperms and their long-distance dispersal. These flowering plants then
co-evolved with pollinating insects, birds, and bats. Seed eating ant species
may also keep granaries for storing food, another social innovation. Social
arrangements became genetically programmed in social insects with altruistic
division of labor as in worker bees and ants bred by Queen and drones. Wilson goes on to show
how inclusive fitness theory and selfish gene theory are incorrect in terms of
eusocial insect genetics of the Queen and her phenotype. The colony can be
likened as a single organism. Wilson
sees individual selection (the Queen and her robots) rather than multilevel
selection (interests of individual furthered by interests of colony) as the
mechanism in social insects. This is still debatable apparently. He goes on to
describe social insect evolution in great detail but the book’s details may be
better remembered with some summary lists, charts, tables, or recaps.
He discusses how natural selection
creates social instincts. He gives an interesting hypothetical example of
alleles for birds to select nesting in oaks and then pines developed when oaks
became scarce so that the selection yields adaptability to changing times and
resource availability. This is an example of micro-evolution where dominance of
a certain allele may prevail when necessary – first for some then in time for
the species as a whole as adapters survive. The example is one of ‘phenotype
plasticity’ based on genotype.
“Natural selection is usually
multilevel: it acts on genes that prescribe targets at more than one level of
biological organization, such as cell and organism, or organism and colony.”
He gives cancer as an example of
when selection at the cellular level conflicts with selection at the level of
the organism.
Wilson goes on to describe kin selection and
inclusive fitness theory in detail and suggest that – reduction of discordance
within a group by group selection (presumably survival of more concordant
groups) is equally plausible. Altruism in eusocial species has often been
desribed in terms of kin selection/inclusive fitness but he suggests that group
selection is equally plausible regardless of kinship. He gives a possible
example as the “floating strategies” of certain species where helpers move
about nests helping in tasks regardless of kinship. Wilson suggests a new theory built in stages:
First individuals come together to build and defend nests or colonies. This
gives advantages to the group. He suggests that close genetic relatedness is a
consequence rather than a cause of eusocial behavior. The second stage is accumulation of other
traits that favor eusociality like care of the brood in the nest. The third
stage is the origin of eusocial alleles through mutation or already mutated
immigrants to the nest. Social genes have yet to be found but there are some
selection forces that lead to alarm calls or chemical signals. These may
develop to the point where different castes in the nest exhibit different
behaviors. The 4th phase is identification of environmental forces
driving group selection. Defense against enemies, predators, parasites, and
rival nests is an example. 5th and finally there is group selection
between colonies which drives life cycles and specialized social behavior.
Next he investigates human nature.
He states that:
“Human nature is the inherited
regularities of mental development common to our species. They are the
“epigenetic rules,” which evolved by the interaction of genetic and cultural
evolution that occurred over a long period in deep prehistory.”
Epigenetic means that the
behaviors are not genetically hardwired or beyond conscious control but are tendencies
based on both genetics and culture. The behaviors are not hardwired like
reflexes but the rules are, he says. The behaviors are learned but we have
predispositions (epigenetic rules) that prepare us to learn them. This process
is an example of gene-culture coevolution. He gives the classic example as the
development of lactose tolerance among human adults some few thousand years
ago. The mutations caused production of lactase, an enzyme that converts sugar
into lactose, to extend beyond infants into adulthood. This was precipitated by
the cultural practice of herding and keeping livestock for milk. Lactose
tolerance is an example of “niche construction.”
He gives another example of an
epigenetic rule as incest taboo and subsequent exogamy (mating out of tribe).
Another is color vocabulary, which rather strangely, it has been determined
that our defined colors are learned rather than inherent, and also based on our
ability to perceive certain wavelengths of light and not others. We learn
colors but we learn them based on our genetic limitations. It has been found
that language affects color classification. Another observation is that the
color red in primates was noticed as it was often fruit to primates in a green
and brown environment. Later it also became the color of sexual advertisement.
Wilson says that we did not invent culture
but the common ancestors of chimps and prehumans did. We merely elaborated on
it. Scientists define culture as “the combination of traits that distinguishes
one group from another.” Culture traits are behaviors invented in a group or
learned from another group. Among animals, chimps and bonobos have the most
advanced cultural repertoire consisting of many culture traits. Humans utilize
their unique long-term memory to vastly elaborate culture. Other animals such
as pigeons and baboons exhibit long-term memory but humans are vastly better at
it. Humans also have the unique ability to build scenarios which may be built
from the memory. Wilson
thinks that “the conscious mind, one of the architecture’s products [complex
inherited architecture of the brain], originated by gene-culture coevolution,
an intricate interplay between genetic and cultural evolution.” Archeological
artifacts suggest that “abstract thought and syntactical language emerged no
later than 70,000 years ago. Since 200,000 years ago hafting of stones onto
spears suggests advanced cognitive ability. Both Neanderthals and Homo sapiens
did this.
The ability to collaborate to
achieve shared goals and intentions required the use of memory and cognitive
abilities. Expressing our intentions and reading the intentions of others
allows us to pursue our collaborative urges. Milestones were possibly the
development of “shared attention” – the ability to pay attention to the same
things as others, and the recognition that our own mental states are shared by
others. These possibly aided the development of language. Language was the
grail of social evolution, he notes. Language is a means to direct the
attention of others and allows us to understand and share intentions, says
linguist Michael Tomasello et al. Animals communicate with warning systems and
some meaning conveyed but the communication is fixed. Human language has many
nuances and abstractions. Language development may be aided as well by
epigenetic rules (prepared learning). An interesting example is that in
tropical climates languages appear to utilize more vowel sounds possibly due to
acoustic efficiency where people spend more time outside and can hear those sounds
better from a distance in competition with other sounds. The genetic basis of
language likely predates language, say current researchers.
Wilson compares rigid traits to traits having
variability and plasticity. An example of plasticity is the uniqueness of human
fingerprints and an example of rigidity is the amount of fingers on a human
hand. Degree of plasticity in gene expression is apparently ruled by natural
selection as well. Regulatory genes rather than coding genes are key to the
evolution of plasticity. This results in more subtle effects on the whole
organism. Wilson
states that:
“The intricacies of gene-culture
coevolution are fundamental to understanding the human condition.”
He examines the origins of
morality and honor seeing them as a product of multilevel selection. “In a
constantly changing world we need the flexibility that only imperfection
provides.” He says that group selection results in in-group altruism.
Individual selection is influenced by competition within a group while group
selection is influenced by competition between groups. He offers a rule in
genetic social evolution, that selfish people win out over altruistic people
but groups of altruists beat groups of selfish humans. Multilevel selection, he
says, is necessary as a way of balancing us between undesirable extremes. The
history of social networks and the modern overlapping nature of multiple
networks make for a complex of group selection processes. The band-sized
network of our hunter-gatherer past is possibly our default network-size
preference. The so-called Golden Rule is fundamental to moral reasoning in most
or all cultures. The relationship to ‘others’ is key to this rule. The
interplay of selfishness and altruism creates an ambiguous or ambivalent state.
The needs and desires of the self seem to always be pitted against the needs
and desires of others. I think Wilson
goes a bit overboard in attributing altruism too much to biological and
cultural influences. Altruism as a goal in itself can be cultivated as a habit
and gradually become more genuine – and eventually spontaneous – according to
Mahayana Buddhist training motifs. Honor as a motivation does seem to be
influenced by both biological and cultural forces aided by the human will.
Confronting injustice takes courage and will and is perhaps fed by a sense of
honor, a desire to be honorable. Selfishness is often seen as dishonorable so
in this way altruism can be motivated by a desire to avoid dishonor. Is this
genuine altruism? It is probably not, so the degree of authentic honor cannot
often be easily determined. Good intentions can also have detrimental
consequences. Ethics can become dogmatic. He gives the example of homophobia.
The origin is likely the notion that sex that is not for reproduction is undesirable
and should be condemned. Homosexuality is often seen as genetic as well as
cultural to a certain extent, yielding possible benefits to the reproductive
success and preservation of human qualities within the group as well as
contributions to human diversity. He says that: “A society that condemns
homosexuality harms itself.” Wilson
suggests that examining the biological origins of morality can be useful and
will tend to verify the more obvious moral choices and tend to reject the ones
based on fear and manipulation such as homophobia, racism, sexism, etc.
Religion initially makes conflict
not between people but between worldviews. Wilson’s analysis of the origin of religion
is interesting. He certainly favors secular science which I think is good but I
also think he neglects some of the value of ritual, psychology, and
philosophical contemplation. He rightly sees modern organized religion as an
expression of tribalism. “The goal of religions is submission to the will and
common good of the tribe.” He thinks Paleolithic peoples reflecting on their
own mortality thought that the dead reappeared in dreams in spirit/dream
worlds. Hallucinogenic drugs likely supported this belief. Working with them
and incorporating them into the creation stories that knit the tribe together
supported the development of mythology. Religion, he notes, has been
incomparable in inspiring the creation of great works of art.
Compared to other animals and
insects our sensory range is rather small, particularly in the realms of taste
and smell. Humans, monkeys, apes, and birds are primarily audio-visual while
other species communicate much with pheromones and chemicals. Of course, now we
can probe the sensory ranges with our scientific tools. Experiments have shown
that the human brain is most aroused by patterns with about 20% redundancy.
This also apparently corresponds to the amount of redundancy and complexity in
many human art pieces. Possibly this is the most complexity the brain can
process instantly so that it is optimized in some sense.
Wilson suggests that the ambiguity in the
human mind created by the conflict of individual selection and group selection
is the key source of art and the humanities. He compares the mechanisms and
rules of science and the humanities. He equates science with the quest for
discovery in concise terms and literature with the use of metaphor which has
the ability to communicate emotional tone. The whole analysis reminds me a bit
of Nietzsche’s Apollo and Dionysus classification of human artistic impulses. Wilson attempts a quick
history of artistic and creativity from early tools to the Paleolithic
explosion of creativity. Flutes over 30,000 years old play just fine and are
well constructed musically. The songs and dances of modern hunter-gathers are
very often about hunting which could be compared to Paleolithic art in which
hunting is the key subject. The songs are also tribal and strengthen memory of
tribal history. He says that to create and perform music is instinctual and I
am inclined to agree mostly to that. Music and language are related. Music may
have been derived from speech as some suggest. Yet is also has beat which
speech does not normally contain.
Wilson emphasizes the biological origins of
culture. We are products of biological evolution. The question of free will –
he suggests is more restricted to biology than is normally considered. Humans
are the largest and most far-reaching eusocial species. He suggests that the
forces of multilevel selection brought us to this godlike state. He considers
group selection as the main driving force of human evolution:
“ … group-level traits, including
cooperativeness, empathy, and patterns of networking, have been found to be
heritable in humans …”
“A basic element of human nature
is that people feel compelled to belong to groups and, having joined, consider
them superior to competing groups.”
The conflicted nature of
motivations and the pull of conscience can be explained by the competing nature
of individual and group selection. Most creation myths favor that tribe as the
chosen of the gods, over other tribes. Ritual helps to organize our tribal
lives and aid in life’s transitions. But modern organized religion is often
divisive and often does not consider well those of other orientations. Studies
have shown that greater interconnection of people through such means as the
internet strengthens their cosmopolitan attitudes – one can only hope. Strengthening
a non-dogmatic approach to life can have positive repercussions such as
allowing us to better work together to solve the problems of the world such as
HIPPO (habitat destruction, invasive species, pollution, overpopulation, and
overharvesting). Our dominance of the planet is shrinking biodiversity. He
stresses the importance of recognizing science as the basis of our belief
systems rather than just a scenario that is competitive with religious
scenarios.
This is a great book that mainly
explores the biological and to a lesser extent the cultural components of
so-called human nature.